Thursday, March 15, 2012

Illinois 3rd in death-reporting errors

When it comes to living folks being reported as dead in the Social Security Administration's Death Master File, Illinois has the third-highest error rate in the country, with 13.2 mistakes per 100,000 people, according to a Scripps Howard News Service report. Only the District of Columbia (17.9) and Louisiana (15.0) make mistakes more often.

In 2007, 461 Illinois residents erroneously were declared dead; in 2008, another 1,675 joined them.

Doug Nguyen, deputy regional communications director for the Social Security Administration in Chicago, said the high rate of mistakes here is because Illinois is not among the 30 states involved in Electronic Death …

Suicidal Behaviours Among Polish Youth in Light of the Following Papers: Suicidal Behaviour in Children and Adolescents. Parts 1 and 2/REPLY

Dear Editor:

It appears from the literature reviews by Dr Margaret M Steele and Dr Tamison Doey1'2 that, both in America and in Europe, a broad definition of attempted suicide (AS) prevails, interchangeably replaced by deliberate self-harm (DSH), which is not, however, informative as to whether any suicidal intentions are associated with a self-harm action.3 Polish studies4 suggest that psychiatric patients with multiple DSH incidents without suicidal intentions differ from AS patients, for example, by personality profiles and emotional intelligence levels, what prompts an immediate assumption that differentiation between these 2 groups of patients is of therapeutic …

Unknown problem interrupts Mars lander's task

The first sample of Martian dirt dumped onto the opening of the Phoenix lander's tiny testing oven failed to reach the instrument and scientists said Saturday they will devote a few days to trying to determine the cause.

Photos released by the University of Arizona team overseeing the mission showed a scoopful of dirt sitting on and around the open oven door after being dumped by the craft's 8-foot robot arm. But none of it made it past a screen and into the tiny chamber, one of eight on the craft designed to heat soil and test gasses for signs of water or organic compounds that could be building blocks for life.

Nothing seems to be wrong with the dirt delivery …

[ TECH BRIEFS ]

Agilent to sell chip unit

Agilent Technologies said it will sell its chip unit to two buyoutfirms for $2.66 billion, as well as sell and spin off other assetsand cut 1,300 jobs as part of a restructuring program. The maker ofelectronic test and measurement products plans to use proceeds fromthe deals for a $4 billion share repurchase program. Kohlberg KravisRoberts & Co. and Silver Lake Partners said their purchase ofAgilent's chip business will create the world's largest privatelyheld independent semiconductor company. The companies said they areequal partners in the venture. Shares of Agilent soared 14.8 percent,up $3.92, to $30.33. Agilent was spun off from …

Wednesday, March 14, 2012

Vietnam War hero was Mississippi Civil Rights Activist

Vietnam War hero was Mississippi Civil Rights Activist

The late Milton Lee Olive III, who, at 18, was the first Black to be awarded a Congressional Medal of Honor during the Vietnam War, preferred to work as a Mississippi civil rights activist but was forced to return to Chicago where he later joined the Army, his cousin revealed Sunday.

Dr. Barbara Penelton, professor and chair of the Teacher Education Department at Bradley University, broke her silence about young Olive, fondly nicknamed "Skipper" by his family and friends.

An educator for 29 years, Penelton said what her cousin wanted more than anything was to fight for voter rights for his people--becoming a …

Frankel aims to live up to hype at Ascot

LONDON (AP) — Named after one of America's greatest racehorse trainers and sired by champion thoroughbred Galileo, Frankel was always destined for big things.

Few, though, could have predicted how great the horse would turn out to be.

The unbeaten 3-year-old colt has captivated the public's imagination in Britain and worldwide with a breakthrough year in 2011, establishing himself as one of the best horses of his or any generation.

The world's top-ranked flat-racing horse will look to live up to his reputation by winning his ninth straight race Saturday — the Queen Elizabeth II Stakes at the inaugural British Champions Day at Ascot.

"Even when you are rating …

Some examples of Internet `hijackings'

Some instances of Internet outages caused by hijacked traffic, as listed by the Department of Homeland Security and other sources:

April 1997: MAI Network Services, an Internet service provider in Virginia, passes bad routing information to Sprint, which relays it, causing widespread outages.

April 1998: An Israeli ISP causes widespread outages.

December 1999: AT&T's server network is hijacked by another ISP.

May 2000: Sprint addresses hijacked by another ISP.

April 2001: Global Internet carrier Flag Telecom hijacks …

Jackson lover's kiss and tell on TV

Karin Stanford, the mother of the Rev. Jesse Jackson's 2-year-olddaughter, appears on ABC's 20/20 tomorrow night and tells host ConnieChung that:

She's offended when people call her Jackson's mistress ... "Idon't see myself as a kept woman ... I work hard."

She's proud of her decision not to have an abortion and thatJackson is pro-choice publicly but "he has a difficult time …

Report: MF Global to seek bankruptcy protection

NEW YORK (AP) — The New York Federal Reserve has suspended any new business with MF Global Holdings amid reports that the securities firm is desperately trying to sell itself. Trading in the company's shares is also halted.

The Wall Street Journal reported Monday that MF Global would seek bankruptcy protection after large investments in sovereign bonds issued by European countries went …

Atletico Madrid beats Villarreal 3-0 in Spain

BARCELONA, Spain (AP) — Atletico Madrid coach Diego Simeone won on his home debut Sunday as his new team beat Villarreal 3-0 in the Spanish league thanks to Radamel Falcao's brace.

Falcao broke through for a dominant Atletico in the 40th minute at Vicente Calderon stadium by scoring from a pass by Adrian Lopez inside the six-yard box.

Falcao scored his 11th league goal from the penalty spot in the 51st after Adrian played the Colombia striker through only for Gonzalo Rodriguez to foul him in the area.

Diego capped the win in the 80th, tapping in a cross from left back Filipe Luis.

"I received a great welcome (from the fans), this is a very special moment both …

Eighteen

Eighteen zine, Jason Stewart (and others), Anxiety Entertainment, $4, 14 Kingsway Cres, St Catherines, ON, L2N 1A6, jayanxiety@yahoo.ca, geocities.com/jayanxiety

We received this zine just a little too late to squeeze it into the horror issue, but better late then never.

Eighteen is a true fanzine. What it lacks in production values is made up for in devotion and content. Volumes two and three of Eighteen each focus on a specific series within the horror genre. Volume two is devoted to The Texas Chainsaw Massacre franchise, with interviews with various actors from the films, reviews of the series and trivia. Volume three is mostly focussed on Dawn of the Dead but it …

Danny Boy the Bird Reunites With Owners

A pet macaw named Danny Boy flew the coop in Carpinteria and was found 15 months later some 80 miles away in Pasadena. Greg and Susan Vasilakos, who have moved from Carpinteria to Colorado, learned days ago that Danny Boy has been found. The bird flew away on Dec. 6, 2006, leaving the couple heartbroken.

They listed Danny Boy on an online bird lost-and-found Web site, but didn't hear anything until this month. The macaw was found flying free March 12 in Pasadena and taken to the Pasadena Humane Society.

The bird was then featured on the society Web site and the Vasilakoses were tracked to their new home …

SUN-TIMES TRIATHLON FACTS

WHAT: 11th annual Chicago Sun-Times Triathlon. WHEN: 6:45 a.m., Aug. 29. DISTANCES: 1.5-kilometer swim, 40-kilometer bike ride, 10-kilometerrun. WHERE: Transition area is at Navy Pier. Swim - out-and-back coursefrom Ohio Street Beach. Bike - Lake Shore Drive between Hollywoodand Oakwood. Run - from transition area south around Soldier Fieldand north to finish on E. Wacker Drive in front of HyattRegency-Chicago. THE FIELD: Led by many of the top pros in theworld, more than 4,000 people will compete as individuals or on relayteams. PRIZE MONEY: Total purse is $40,000, with first-place manand woman each receiving $5,000. EXPO: Chicago Consumer Multi-Sport Expo will run 4-9:30 p.m. nextFriday and 10 a.m.-6 p.m. Aug. 28 at the Hyatt Regency-Chicago, 151E. Wacker Drive. It is free and open to the public. POSTRACE: Awards ceremony and party, 10 a.m.-4 p.m. Aug. 29 in plazaat Hyatt Regency-Chicago. INFORMATION: For more details, call the race hotline at (312)404-2292.

U.S. Death Sentences Drop to 30-Year Low

The number of death sentences handed out in the United States dropped in 2006 to the lowest level since capital punishment was reinstated 30 years ago, reflecting what some experts say is a growing fear that the criminal justice system will make a tragic and irreversible mistake.

Executions fell, too, to the fewest in a decade.

"The death penalty is on the defensive," said Richard Dieter, director of the Death Penalty Information Center, a Washington organization that looks at problems with the capital punishment system.

Death sentences fell in 2006 to 114 or fewer, according to an estimate from the group. That is down from 128 in 2005, and even lower than the 137 sentences the year after the U.S. Supreme Court reinstated the death penalty in 1976. It is also down sharply from the high of 317 in 1996.

A total of 53 executions were carried out in 2006, down from 60 in 2005. Executions over the past three decades peaked at 98 in 1999.

Among the many causes given by prosecutors, lawyers and death penalty critics: the passage of more state laws that allow juries to impose life without parole; an overall drop in violent crime; and a reluctance among some authorities to pursue the death penalty because of the high costs of prosecuting a capital case.

But above all, many said, is the possibility of a mistake, made dramatically clear in recent years. Since the death penalty was reinstated, 123 people have been freed from death row after significant questions were raised about their convictions - 14 of them through DNA testing, according to the Death Penalty Information Center.

"The fact is they've gotten a lot of the wrong guys," said Deborah Fleischaker, director of the American Bar Association's Death Penalty Moratorium Implementation Project. "There's no question that has, in the public, created a lot of doubt about how the death penalty is working."

The turn away from the ultimate punishment also reflects a changing sentiment among juries and prosecutors, too, said Arthur Green, a prosecutor in Bessemer, Ala., outside of Birmingham. He said he considers the risk of executing an innocent person in deciding whether to pursue the death penalty.

"That's one reason I don't do it, except in very, very rare circumstances - one, that I'm convinced he or she did it, and number two, it's a horrible crime," Green said. He has sought and won two capital cases since becoming district attorney in 2001.

Thirty-seven of the 38 states that have the death penalty on their books now also allow for life without parole. Texas enacted such a law in 2005. Life-without-parole laws give another option to jurors who fear that the death penalty is the only way to keep a killer from getting out on the streets again.

The death penalty has also received more scrutiny from lawmakers around the country and the courts.

Illinois is in the seventh year of its moratorium on executions, and executions are effectively halted in New York because of a 2004 court ruling.

Also, questions about whether lethal injection is inhumane have put executions on hold in nine states - Arkansas, California, Delaware, Florida, Maryland, Missouri, New Jersey, Ohio and South Dakota.

This week in New Jersey, a special commission recommended that the state become the first to abolish the death penalty legislatively since 1976, citing "increasing evidence that the death penalty is inconsistent with evolving standards of decency."

Backers of capital punishment say support for the death penalty remains strong, despite the drop-off in death sentences.

"It's a refinement. I don't think it's an abandonment of the death penalty, but a recognition that the death penalty should be reserved for the worst of the worst," said Joshua Marquis, district attorney in Clatsop County, Ore., and a vice president of the National District Attorneys Association.

"From prosecutors, there's a more discriminating attitude about which cases to bring," he said. "Juries have been much more picky about allocating the death penalty and I think that's an appropriate thing to do."

A Gallup poll in May found that two-thirds of Americans 18 and older support the death penalty. But when asked which is the better penalty for murder, roughly half said life without parole and about half said the death penalty.

Tuesday, March 13, 2012

Panel Hears Climate 'Spin' Allegations

WASHINGTON - Federal scientists have been pressured by the White House to play down global warming, advocacy groups testified Tuesday at the Democrats' first investigative hearing since taking control of Congress.

The hearing focused on allegations that White House officials for years have micromanaged the government's climate programs and has closely controlled what scientists have been allowed to tell the public.

"It appears there may have been an orchestrated campaign to mislead the public about climate change," said Rep. Henry Waxman, D-Calif. Waxman is chairman of the Oversight and Government Reform Committee and a critic of the Bush administration's environmental policies, including its views on climate.

Climate change also was a leading topic in the Senate, where presidential contenders for 2008 lined up at a hearing called by Sen. Barbara Boxer. They expounded - and at times tried to outdo each other - on why they believed Congress must act to reduce heat-trapping "greenhouse" gases.

"This is a problem whose time has come," Sen. Hillary Rodham Clinton, D-N.Y., proclaimed.

"This is an issue over the years whose time has come," echoed Sen. John McCain, R-Ariz.

Sen. Barack Obama, D-Ill., said "for decades far too many have ignored the warning" about climate change. "Will we look back at today and say this was the moment we took a stand?"

At the House hearing, two private advocacy groups produced a survey of 279 government climate scientists showing that many of them say they have been subjected to political pressure aimed at downplaying the climate threat. Their complaints ranged from a challenge to using the phrase "global warming" to raising uncertainty on issues on which most scientists basically agree, to keeping scientists from talking to the media.

The survey and separate interviews with scientists "has brought to light numerous ways in which U.S. federal climate science has been filtered, suppressed and manipulated in the last five years," Francesca Grifo, a senior scientist at the Union of Concerned Scientists, told the committee.

Grifo's group, along with the Government Accountability Project, which helps whistle-blowers, produced the report.

Drew Shindell, a climate scientist with NASA's Goddard Institute for Space Studies, said that climate scientists frequently have been dissuaded from talking to the media about their research, though NASA's restrictions have been eased.

Prior to the change, interview requests of climate scientists frequently were "routed through the White House" and then turned away or delayed, said Shindell. He described how a news release on his study forecasting a significant warming in Antarctica was "repeatedly delayed, altered and watered down" at the insistence of the White House.

Some Republican members of the committee questioned whether science and politics ever can be kept separate.

"I am no climate-change denier," said Rep. Tom Davis of Virginia, the top Republican on the committee, but he questioned whether "the issue of politicizing science has itself become politicized."

"The mere convergence of politics and science does not itself denote interference," said Davis.

Administration officials were not called to testify. In the past the White House has said it has only sought to inject balance into reports on climate change. President Bush has acknowledged concerns about global warming, but he strongly opposes mandatory caps of greenhouse gas emissions, arguing that approach would be too costly.

Roger Pielke Jr., a political scientist at the University of Colorado who was invited by GOP lawmakers, said "the reality is that science and politics are intermixed."

Pielke maintained that "scientific cherry picking" can be found on both sides of the climate debate. He took a swipe at the background memorandum Waxman had distributed and maintained that it exaggerated the scientific consensus over the impact of climate change on hurricanes.

Waxman and Davis agreed the administration had not been forthcoming in providing documents to the committee that would shed additional light on allegations of political interference in climate science.

"We know that the White House possesses documents that contain evidence of an attempt by senior administration officials to mislead the public by injecting doubt into the science of global warming and minimize the potential danger," said Waxman, adding that he is "not trying to obtain state secrets."

At Boxer's Senate hearing, her predecessor as chairman of the Environment and Public Works Committee, Sen. James Inhofe, R-Okla., had his own view of the science.

There is "no convincing scientific evidence" that human activity is causing global warming, declared Inhofe, who once called global warming a hoax. "We all know the Weather Channel would like to have people afraid all the time."

"I'll put you down as skeptical," replied Boxer.

---

Associated Press writer Erica Werner contributed to this report.

---

On the Net:

House committee: http://oversight.house.gov

Pro-Sandinista envoy is transferred by pope

VATICAN CITY (UPI) Pope John Paul II yesterday named a Malteseprelate to serve as papal nuncio to Nicaragua, succeeding a veterandiplomat reportedly transferred because he was considered toofriendly to the Sandinistas.

Monsignor Paolo Giglio, 59, who began his career as a Vaticandiplomat in Nicaragua and is now charge d'affaires in Taiwan, alsowill be nuncio to Honduras.

A nuncio is the permanent official representative of the pope toa foreign government.

The Vatican announced without comment Tuesday that the presentnuncio to Nicaragua and Honduras, Archbishop Andrea Cordero Lanza diMontezemolo, 60, would become envoy to Uruguay.

The Turin newspaper La Stampa reported that Miguel CardinalObando y Bravo, the archbishop of Managua, had bluntly asked the popeat a recent audience to choose between himself and Cordero Lanza diMontezemolo, who advocated a dialogue with the Nicaraguan government.

La Stampa said the main reason for the Italian diplomat'stransfer was his clash with Obando y Bravo, a leading critic of theSandinistas.

The pope previously showed his support for Obando y Bravo'santi-Sandinista stand by making him Nicaragua's first cardinal lastMay.

Giglio, born in Malta in 1927 and ordained there in 1952,studied theology, canon law and diplomacy and also has a diploma asan archivist. He speaks English, Italian, Spanish and French.

The Vatican chose not to appoint another nuncio to Taiwan topave the way for establishing closer relations with Peking, Vaticansources have said.

Fletcher gives Texas edge over Detroit

Texas and Detroit are off to surprising starts this season -surprisingly good for the Rangers and surprisingly bad for theTigers.

Scott Fletcher went 3-for-3 and scored twice and rookie KevinBrown pitched a seven-hitter Saturday as the Rangers won their seventh straight game, 4-1 over the Tigers at Detroit.

The Rangers' 9-1 start is their best since the franchise movedto Texas 17 years ago. Detroit (2-6) is off to its worst start since1980.

"Walter Alston used to say the games now are just as importantas the games in September," Texas manager Bobby Valentine said. "SoI'm looking at this as a good time to win."

It also is the first time the Rangers have won their first fourgames against Detroit since 1975. Texas was 4-8 against Detroit ineach of the previous two seasons.

Detroit manager Sparky Anderson said it's too early to evaluatehis team.

"Anything close to a 20-20 record after 40 games and I'll behappy," he said. "After 40 games, a team will give you a good ideahow you will do in a year."

Brown (1-0), Texas' top 1986 draft choice who was 1-1 in fourSeptember starts, struck out four and walked one in pitching hissecond complete game.

"We feel we have the people to do the job now," Fletcher said."We've got a lot of confidence in each other."

Frank Tanana (0-2), who had won his previous six decisionsagainst Texas, allowed four runs on nine hits in seven innings.

Royals 10, Blue Jays 5: Pat Tabler's bases-loaded single keyeda three-run rally in the seventh inning that helped lift Kansas Cityover host Toronto.

Tabler, 38-for-66 with 90 RBI in his career with the basesloaded, singled home one run for a 6-5 lead. The Royals scored fourtimes in the ninth on a three-run double by Danny Tartabull and anRBI single by Bo Jackson.

The Blue Jays scored five runs in the first inning and chasedBret Saberhagen. Reliever Luis Aquino (2-0) pitched 6 1/3 scorelessinnings, giving up three hits.

Orioles 12, Red Sox 4: Larry Sheets and Brady Anderson hitthree-run homers and Baltimore took advantage of Mike Boddicker'swildness to win at Boston.

Boddicker (1-1) allowed five runs on seven hits with threewalks, a wild pitch and a hit batter in 5 1/3 innings. He lost forthe second time in eight starts at Fenway Park since being tradedfrom Baltimore to Boston last July 29.

Brewers 5, Indians 1: Rob Deer hit two home runs to helpMilwaukee beat visiting Cleveland, ending Tom Candiotti's eight-gamewinning streak over two seasons.

Deer hit a two-run homer in the sixth inning for a 4-1 lead. Heconnected for his fourth home run of the season in the eighth againstCandiotti (1-1), who had not lost since last July 22. Chris Bosio(2-0) earned his second victory over Cleveland this season.

Angels 9, Mariners 2: Chuck Finley (2-1) scattered nine singlesas California won at Seattle. Chili Davis hit a three-run homer inthe ninth inning and Devon White and Jack Howell hit solo home runsfor the Angels.

Twins-Yankees postponed: Saturday's game between Minnesota andNew York was rained out. The game was rescheduled for Aug. 1 or 3.

Adolescent and young adult male hockey players: Nutrition knowledge and education

Abstract/Resume

Athletes often have inadequate nutrition knowledge and poor nutritional habits, which can have a negative impact on athletic performance. This study assessed the nutrition knowledge of competitive adolescent and young adult male hockey players, and examined the impact of a nutrition intervention program, Sport Nutrition for the Athletes of Canada (SNAC). Before the intervention, nutrition knowledge was tested in 175 adolescent and young adult male hockey players. The intervention was provided as part of a hockey camp curriculum and was based on the SNAC workbook, which emphasizes achieving a balanced diet with adequate energy and fluid intake. After the intervention, nutrition knowledge was assessed in a subgroup of 33 hockey players. The pre-intervention nutrition knowledge score was 45% (n = 175), which suggests this population had little sport nutrition knowledge. Nutrition knowledge scores two weeks after the intervention showed no meaningful improvement in the subgroup. The results of this study suggest that the SNAC nutrition intervention program offered under the conditions of this study, did not effectively improve nutrition knowledge in adolescent and young adult male hockey players. (Can J Diet Prac Res 1999; 60:166-169)

Les sportifs ont souvent des connaissances en nutrition insuffisantes et des habitudes nutritionnelles qui laissent a desirer, et leur performance peut en souffrir. Cette etude avait pour but d'evaluer les connaissances en nutrition de joueurs de hockey de competition et d'examiner les effets d'un programme d'intervention en nutrition, Sport et nutrition pour les athletes du Canada (SNAC). Les connaissances en nutrition ont ete testees avant l'intervention chez 175 adolescents et jeunes adultes de sexe masculin. L'intervention a ete appliquee chez les 175 joueurs au cours d'un camp de hockey. Elle etait basee sur le guide du programme SNAC avec une insistance particuliere sur la consommation d'une alimentation equilibree comportant un apport adequat en energie et en liquide. Les connaissances en nutrition ont ete evaluees apres l'intervention chez un sous-groupe de 33 joueurs. Le score de depart pour les connaissances en nutrition etait de 45% (n = 175), ce qui porte A croire que cette population possedait peu de connaissances en nutrition liees au sport avant l'intervention. Deux semaines apres l'intervention, les connaissances ne s'etaient pas ameliorees de facon significative dans le sous-groupe. Les resultats revelent que le programme d'intervention SNAC n'a pas ete efficace pour ameliorer les connaissances en nutrition chez des adolescents et de jeunes adultes joueurs de hockey. (Rev can prat rech dietet 1999; 60:166-169)

INTRODUCTION

Nutrition is one of many factors contributing to optimal athletic performance (1). However, many athletes lack nutrition knowledge and have poor nutrition habits (2-4). The impact of nutrition on sport performance has received considerable recognition. Because athletes are continually seeking a competitive edge, they may be likely to seek nutrition knowledge and adopt healthy eating behaviours (2, 3).

In 1991 the Sport Medicine and Science Council of Canada and the Beef Information Centre designed an interactive sport nutrition education program, Sport Nutrition for the Athletes of Canada (SNAC) (5). The purpose was to improve adolescent athletes' knowledge of basic nutrition and to help them use that information to attain and maintain peak performance. The first evaluation of SNAC showed that the program increased nutrition knowledge significantly, but did not change attitudes or behaviour over the short term (6).

Hockey places high physical demands on the players (1). Adult hockey players require 5,000 to 6,000 kcal a day during the season and can lose up to 4.54 kg (10 lb) of fluid during a game or workout if fluid replacement is inadequate (1). Children and adolescents have proportionately greater energy and nutrient requirements for support during periods of rapid growth and development (7). Dehydration is one of the most common factors that impairs athletic performance, and children and adolescents are even more susceptible to overheating because of their lower sweating capacity (8). Because of the physical demands of hockey, nutrition education may be important to improve young athletes' nutritional status and consequently enhance athletic performance. Currently there is minimal information on adolescent hockey players' nutrition knowledge.

The purpose of the present study was to assess SNAC's effectiveness at improving adolescent and young adult hockey players' nutrition knowledge. The SNAC program was to be deemed effective if knowledge improved meaningfully and significantly after intervention.

METHODS Subjects:

All participants in a summer hockey school were recruited. Nutrition education was part of the hockey school curriculum. Total enrollment consisted of 181 competitive male hockey players aged ten to 21.The University of Alberta Faculty of Agriculture, Forestry and Home Economics Human Ethics Review Committee approved the study protocol. Although 175 athletes participated in the nutrition education program and completed a pre-intervention nutrition knowledge questionnaire, unpredictable circumstances resulted in an extremely high attrition rate. Thus, only a subgroup (n = 33) were given both the pretest and a post-test to assess their nutrition knowledge after intervention. Nutrition education:

Nutrition intervention took the form of four modules provided to the athletes (n = 175) in two separate one-hour periods, one week apart. Each nutrition education session was administered to approximately 30-40 hockey players at a time. The modules comprised lectures, large-group discussions, video presentations, and group and individual activities. The modules focused on achieving a balanced diet with adequate energy, carbohydrate, and fluid intake before, during, and after intense exercise. All information was based on Sport Nutrition for the Athletes of Canada: Workbook forAthletes (5).

Nutrition knowledge: A modified version of the SNAC questionnaire was used to assess nutrition knowledge before and after the intervention. The pre-intervention knowledge test was administered before the first education module. The post-intervention knowledge test was administered one week after the last intervention module. The total intervention time was two weeks from the pretest to the post-test. The subjects' nutrition knowledge was assessed using the total scores of the test questionnaire: the highest possible score was 45.

Content validity of the original SNAC questionnaire knowledge section was established by experts in nutrition education, sport nutrition, and questionnaire design review, who reviewed and approved all questions (5). The original questionnaire was tested with 34 adolescent athletes to ensure comprehension. Also, this questionnaire has previously been shown to be reliable (r = 0.93) (5).

The nutrition knowledge questionnaire used to assess our subjects' nutrition knowledge was modified from the original; 45 of the original 62 questions were retained. Redundant questions were omitted because of time constraints. The reliability of the shorter, modified questionnaire was not evaluated. However, we assumed that its reliability would not differ from that of the original test, as only repetitive questions were omitted. The questionnaire consisted of true-or-false, multiple choice, and "choose all those that apply" questions (e.g., "Place a check beside all the foods that contain carbohydrates").

Demographic data: Age and the number of years playing hockey were determined by questionnaire before the pre-intervention questionnaire was administered. These variables were assessed because of their possible relation to nutrition knowledge test scores. For example, older and more experienced players may have had more nutrition education that could result in higher test scores. Data analysis: Data were analyzed with the Statistical Package for Social Sciences II (SPSS/PC+) software statistics program (version 4.0, SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, 1990). Paired-difference t-tests were used to determine the mean difference of pre- and post-test scores. Pearson correlation analysis was used to examine the relationships between knowledge scores, and the number of years involved in the sport.

DISCUSSION

The low mean nutrition knowledge test score before any education (20.4 4.8 out of a possible score of 45 [n = 175]) demonstrates the athletes' limited nutrition knowledge before intervention. Although the mean scores increased in the subgroup's post-tests, knowledge did not improve meaningfully (scores were still < 50%) as a result of the intervention. This could have been a result of the large student-to-instructor ratio; limited time for questions, clarification, and review; and the physical and social environment, which was not conducive to learning. Previous research has shown that small-group education or individual counselling is ideal for delivering nutrition education (9).

Previous studies have shown that increasing age, educational level, and time in the sport were related to increased knowledge scores (2,3,10,11). This was not demonstrated in the current study. The low pre-intervention nutrition knowledge test scores revealed the need for an effective nutrition education program. These results are consistent with those of other studies of pre-intervention nutrition knowledge, although different test instruments were used. In a study of high school wrestlers, the mean nutrition knowledge test score before education was 58% (12). In the same study, 75 male college track, baseball, and football athletes scored below 50% on a nutrition knowledge test (12). Barr (2) compared the nutrition knowledge of female university students and varsity athletic team members. Female varsity athletes had low levels of general and sports-related nutrition knowledge, which were similar to those of female non-athletes. These findings suggest that:

athletes do not aspire to a higher level of nutrition knowl

edge to attain a competitive edge,

they are obtaining incorrect nutrition information from poor sources, or

nutrition education programs are limited and not widely available.

Limitations of the current study included lack of participants for both pre- and post-intervention testing, the shortterm nature of the assessment, lack of a control group, and the environment in which the intervention was delivered. The lack of a control group made it difficult to assess whether the improved post-evaluation test score was related to increased knowledge or whether the participants learned from the pre-intervention test. In conclusion, nutrition intervention based on the SNAC education program offered under the conditions of this study, was not an effective means of delivering sport nutrition information to large groups of adolescent and young adult male hockey players.

RELEVANCE TO PRACTICE

For nutrition education to be effective, it should be thoroughly planned and include a pre-assessment of the learner's knowledge and competencies. Small-group discussions are most effective for nutrition education because they provide less distraction and more time for questions, clarification, and review (13). The content and learning activities should be appropriate; programs for adolescent and young adult athletes should emphasize motivational, simple, and practical "how to" knowledge that can be applied to athletic performance (9). To facilitate learning, the social and physical environment should be bright, quiet, comfortable, and free of distractions (14).

Because the majority of athletes obtain sport nutrition information from their coaches and trainers (10,11), these people must have a good understanding of nutrition so that they can provide accurate information. Successful nutrition education programs should therefore include coaches and trainers.

[Reference]

References

[Reference]

1. Burns J, Dugan L. Working with professional athletes in the rink: the evolution of a nutrition program for an NHL team. Intl J Sport Nutr 1994;4:132-4.

[Reference]

2. Barr S. Nutrition knowledge of female varsity athletes and university students. J Am Diet Assoc 1987;87:1660-4.

3. Perron M, Endres J. Knowledge, attitudes and dietary practices of female athletes. J Am Diet Assoc 1985;85:573-6.

4. Shoaf L, McClellan P, Birskovich K. Nutrition knowledge, interests and information sources of male athletes. J Nutr Ed 1986;18(6):243-5. 5. Caulfield CA. Sport nutrition for the athletes of Canada: workbook for athletes. Sport Medicine Council of Canada, 1991. 6. Chuey PM. Sport nutrition for the athletes of Canada (SNAC): does it influence the nutrition knowledge, attitudes and behaviours of young athletes? MSc Thesis, University of Saskatchewan, 1994.

[Reference]

7. Thompson JL. Energy balance in young athletes. Intl J Sport Nutr 1998;1:160-74.

8. Bar-Or O. Climate and the exercising child: a review. Intl J Sports Med 1980;1:53-65.

[Reference]

9. Rush IC The school nurse and sport nutrition for the adolescent athlete. J School Health 1990;60(8):425-7.

10. Fredrick L, Hawkins S. A comparison of nutrition knowledge and attitudes, dietary practices, and bone densities of postmenopausal women, female college athletes and non-athletic women. J Am Diet Assoc 1992;92:299-305.

[Reference]

11. Wta B, Stombaugh I, Buch J. Nutrition knowledge and eating practices: health and performance concerns of young women athletes and their coaches. J Phys Ed Recr Dance 1995;66(3):36-41.

12. Steen S, McKinney S. Nutrition assessment of college wrestlers. Phys Sports Med 1986;14(11):100-16.

13. Glanz K, Rudd J. Views of theory, research and practice: a survey of nutrition education and consumer behaviour professionals. J Nutr Ed 1993;25(5):269-75.

14. Glanz K, Erikson MP. Individual and community models for dietary behaviour change. J Nutr Ed 1993;25(2):80-6.

[Author Affiliation]

KAREN J. READING, BASc; LINDA J. McCARGAR, PhD, RD; BARBARA J. MARRIAGE, MSc, RD, Department of Agricultural, Food and Nutritional Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB

Forecasts

The humidity is coming back. Tonight, partly cloudy with some fogdeveloping. Low: 62. Friday, partly to mostly cloudy. Humid and verywarm. High: 82.

Lisa Teachman

Yeager wins Dunbar mayor's office, most incumbents voted out

Jack Yeager won't be Dunbar's interim mayor for long.

He will keep the job for at least the next four years, afterwinning 72 percent of the votes in Tuesday's general election.

Yeager was appointed interim mayor late last year after formermayor Roger Wolfe was removed from the post.

After his first campaign for office, Yeager finished the raceTuesday with 784 votes, compared to Republican candidate Rev. LannyCoberly Sr.'s 290 votes.

Coberly congratulated Yeager on his way out of the county'svoter's registration office, where the votes were counted Tuesdaynight.

"It was a good, clean race," Coberly said. "There was nonegativity on either side. I stand ready to help if I'm needed."

Coberly, a minister at a Dunbar church, said he would considerrunning for office again in the future.

Yeager, a professor-emeritus at Marshall University's GraduateCollege and retired brigadier general of the West Virginia AirNational Guard, has lived in Dunbar for 41 years.

"It's a new day in the city," Yeager said. "We lost some goodcouncil members but we've also gained a few good council members."

Only two incumbents on council kept their seats.

One lost by just two votes.

In the Ward 3 race for two spots on council, incumbent EvelynColeman, a Democrat, lost her bid. She came in third with 51 votes.

Claiming the seats were newcomer Connie Thompson, a Republican,who won 85 votes, and incumbent Democrat Tom Bailey, who captured 53votes.

Coleman had served on city council since she was appointed inOctober 2004 after her husband, Councilman John Coleman, died.

This is the first time Thompson, 46, has dabbled in politics.

"I've never done anything like this before," said Thompson, whosaid she gained experience dealing with the public while working inthe food service industry. "I just want Dunbar to shine and to be agood community again."

In Ward 1, two new councilmen will take office next month.

Republican Mike Scipio, who came out ahead with 285 votes, hadserved on council until he lost his seat in the previous generalelection.

Democrat Rick "R.J." Fields grabbed the second seat in the wardwith 199 votes. Incumbent Janet Nelson, Democrat, was ousted by a 17-vote margin. She gathered 182 votes.

Councilman Fred Chiles lost his seat in that ward during theprimary election.

Ward 2 Councilman Fred Slaymaker, a Democrat, also lost his seatTuesday by a narrow margin. Slaymaker got 186 votes, but lost by 10votes to Republican Ross "Pepper" Harrison, who took 196 votes.Newcomer Bill Dunlap, a Democrat, won the most votes in that wardwith 210.

"I'm okay," Slaymaker said. "They've got Pepper now, and he's agood man. As long as the people who won serve the city well I'll behappy."

Slaymaker served on city council for four years and said hisbiggest accomplishment would be the new police bicycle unit startingin the city in the coming weeks.

Slaymaker and several officers displayed one of the department'stwo new Cannondale police bicycles at Monday evening's councilmeeting.

There were no surprises in Ward 4 where Councilman EveretteSullivan, a Democrat, was unopposed, along with newcomer DemocratGail L. Harper. Councilwoman Connie Boardman gave up her seat inthat ward to make an unsuccessful run for city clerk. She lost inthe primary.

Carroll Dunlap took the position of city clerk Tuesday afterreceiving 799 votes. Dunlap, a Democrat, was unopposed in thegeneral election.

Dunlap, the father of Bill Dunlap, is a Verizon retiree and has20 years of experience in city government, including several yearsas a city councilman.

"I want to see Dunbar move forward," the elder Dunlap said."We've got big plans for the city."

Of the 5,452 registered voters in Dunbar, 1,108 - a little morethan 20 percent - turned out to vote Tuesday.

New council members will be sworn in later this month andofficially take office July 1.

Deficits in Other-Race Face Recognition: No Evidence for Encoding-Based Effects

The other-race effect (ORE) in face recognition is typically observed in tasks which require long-term memory. Several studies, however, have found the effect early in face encoding (Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991; Walker & Hewstone, 2006). In 6 experiments, with over 300 participants, we found no evidence that the recognition deficit associated with the ORE reflects deficits in immediate encoding. In Experiment 1, with a study-to-test retention interval of 4 min, participants were better able to recognise White faces, relative to Asian faces. Experiment 1 also validated the use of computer-generated faces in subsequent experiments. In Experiments 2 through 4, performance was virtually identical to Asian and White faces in match-to-sample, immediate recognition. In Experiment 5, decreasing target-foil similarity and disrupting the retention interval with trivia questions elicited a re-emergence of the ORE. Experiments 6A and 6B replicated this effect, and showed that memory for Asian faces was particularly susceptible to distraction; White faces were recognised equally well, regardless of trivia questions during the retention interval. The recognition deficit in the ORE apparently emerges from retention or retrieval deficits, not differences in immediate perceptual processing.

Keywords: face recognition, other-race effect, memory

In the past 50 years, research has consistently shown that people are extraordinary face processors, capable of recognising hundreds of faces across lengthy stretches of time (e.g., Bahrick, Bahrick, & Wittlinger, 1975). Despite such expertise, research also has consistently shown that people are relatively poor at learning and remembering faces from members of other racial groups (for a review, see Meissner & Brigham, 2001). This other-race effect (ORE; also, own-race bias, cross-race effect, etc.) has been welldocumented under a variety of paradigms and is reliably observed across social and racial groups (e.g., Anthony, Copper, & Mullen, 1992; Bothwell, Brigham, & Malpass, 1989; Chance, Goldstein, & McBride, 1975; Ferguson, Rhodes, Lee, & Sriram, 2001; Ng & Lindsay, 1994). The effect has been documented in memory experiments with relatively short (e.g., 2 min) retention intervals (O'Toole, Deffenbacher, Valentin, & Abdi, 1994), and with retention intervals extending into days (Slone, Brigham, & Meissner, 2000). Such recognition differences do not apparently arise from concrete physiological differences amongst faces of different races (Goldstein, 1979), nor do they reflect negative racial attitudes (Slone et al., 2000; Swope, 1994). More interesting, race effects are not limited to long-term memory deficits; they also have immediate perceptual consequences, as researchers have found visual search asymmetries (Levin, 1996, 2000), differences in perceived lightness (Levin & Banaji, 2006), and differences in perceived colour (Papesh & Goldinger, 2009), all as a function of depicted race. Race effects encompass myriad perceptual and memory effects, all of which hinge on an interaction between the race of the observer and the race (either subjective or objective) of the studied face.

The factor most commonly found to contribute to the presence and magnitude of the ORE is a person's relative degree of interracial contact. Contact hypotheses have been proposed in various forms and generally have been supported (e.g., Allport, 1954, as cited in Walker & Hewstone, 2006; Goldstein & Chance, 1980; Hancock & Rhodes, 2008, but see Brigham & Barkowitz, 1978, for an exception). All make similar claims, that recognition ability will be poor for members of one race who have had limited exposure to members of another race (Ng & Lindsay, 1994). This is an intuitively logical hypothesis. Gibson (1969), for instance, suggested that growing experience in any domain will lead to perceptual expertise, an increased ability to extract information necessary for accurate processing and recognition. This is also true of faces: As experience with a class of faces increases, so does the ability to quickly and accurately extract the information necessary for successful identification.

However, what information is extracted from faces, allowing such robust recognition? Researchers have consistently found that greater perceptual expertise corresponds to greater reliance on configurai information from faces, relative to reliance on featural information (see Rakover, 2002, for an overview). Briefly, experts in any perceptual domain are more likely to process stimuli with emphasis on the Gestalt, the whole stimulus, rather than on serial consideration of its constituent parts (Diamond & Carey, 1986; Gauthier & Bukach, 2007; but see Robbins & McKone, 2007). For faces, configurai information is contained within the spatial arrangement of the various features and their interrelations. When processing own-race faces, people have been found to rely most heavily on configurai cues, whereas encoding other-race faces relies more heavily on featural processing, or the serial analysis of individual structures (Farah, Wilson, Tanaka, & Drain, 1998; Hancock & Rhodes, 2008; Michel, Caldara, & Rossion, 2006; Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006; Rhodes, Tan, Brake, & Taylor, 1989; Tanaka, Kiefer, & Bukach, 2004). It is commonly argued that such initial processing differences underlie the deficits seen in recognition memory tasks.

Typically, the ORE is expressed as better recognition memory for own-race faces, relative to other-race faces. This appears as both a decrease in sensitivity and as a bias shift, with increased false alarms to other-race faces. Many theories have been proposed to explain this bias, including multidimensional face-space frameworks (Byatt & Rhodes, 2004; Valentine, 1991; Valentine & Endo, 1992), contact hypotheses, feature-selection hypotheses (cf. Diamond & Carey, 1986; Hancock & Rhodes, 2008; Levin, 1996; Rakover, 2002), and notions of perceptual expertise (Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991; Rakover, 2002), wherein members of one race are better able to extract the necessary individuating information from own-race faces, relative to other-race faces. Recent research, however, suggests that the ORE may not simply reflect long-term memory, but also differences in early perceptual processing. Race effects in immediate face perception are generally construed as differences in depths of processing: Other-race faces tend to be processed with emphasis on categorical information, whereas own-race faces are individuated more thoroughly, beginning at the earliest stages of processing (MacLin & Malpass, 2001; Ostrom, Carpenter, Sedikides, & Li, 1993; Sporer, 2001, but see Hayward, Rhodes, & Schwaninger, 2008, for evidence of an own-race advantage in both configurai and component processing). Levin (1996, 2000) suggested that racial information is processed as a basic visual feature (cf. Triesman & Gelade, 1980), defined by its presence (as in other-race faces) or absence (as in own-race faces). According to Levin's theory, faces from different races can be thoroughly processed, with the same degree of individuation as own-race faces. Such complete appreciation, however, only occurs when people are motivated to do so. Oftentimes, people "stop encoding" beyond the initial processing of the basic race feature. Such immediate perceptual effects of other-race face processing (e.g., Levin, 1996) are relevant, as they may predict that deficits will arise in immediate recognition memory, the focus of our investigation.

Lindsay et al. (1991) investigated immediate identification deficits in other-race face memory. Using faces with matched foils and a match-to-sample task, they found that own-race recognition was more accurate than other-race recognition, at least amongst White participants. The magnitude of their effect correlated negatively with the amount of interracial experience reported by their participants (i.e., people with greater experience were less susceptible to the ORE). Since that study, several other researchers have reported that faces are encoded differently based on race (Eberhardt, Dasgupta, & Banaszynski, 2003), such that memory deficits are immediately observable. Walker and Tanaka (2003) and Walker and Hewstone (2006) morphed faces along a biracial continuum and found evidence for an early perceptual effect, wherein own-race faces led to superior "same/different" discrimination. Such results are interesting, as they suggest that recognition memory deficits in the ORE may arise immediately, and are not the result of long-term memory processes, a hypothesis we examine in our experiments.

Overview

In the present study, we sought to replicate the match-to-sample results of Lindsay et al. (1991) with more stringently controlled stimuli. Lindsay et al. concluded their article by noting that otherrace asymmetries in face recognition cannot be adequately explained unless both races are tested or, as they implied, researchers develop an objective measure of facial similarity. We have done the latter, by creating computer-generated faces, which we validated in Experiment 1. We then conducted a series match-tosample experiments, wherein people saw a prime face, then tried to select it moments later in an AX task with itself (as a target) and a matched foil. By using computerized faces, we precisely controlled the physical similarity of targets (A) and foils (X) within and across races. In this methodology, immediate face encoding can be evaluated across depicted races, without confounds from overall similarity (or confusability) of the faces as a group. In every trial, people tried to discriminate a target face from a distorted version of itself.1 Levels of distortion were equal across own- and other-race faces.

Using the computerized faces, we find that deficits in immediate face recognition are difficult (we refrain from saying "impossible") to observe, and instead emerge in recognition tests following retention intervals during which immediate memory is disrupted. We achieved this by distracting participants during the retention interval, perhaps forcing them to rely on relatively long-term memory processes, changing the task from immediate to delayed recognition. Across experiments, we examined the time course of recognition deficits associated with the ORE by manipulating retention time and/or target-foil similarity. All experiments contrasted Asian and White face recognition. In Experiment 1, we tested whether participants experienced the classic ORE when given relatively lengthy encoding periods (3,200 ms per face), followed by a retention interval of several minutes. In Experiments 2 through 4, we varied target-foil similarity (and other factors) hi match-to-sample tasks, seeking to determine whether the ORE can be elicited following briefer wait periods. Experiments 5 and 6 tested recognition accuracy for White and Asian faces, with reduced target-foil similarity and distracting questions presented during the retention intervals. Of the 368 participants included in the following experiments (including experiments reported in footnotes), 86% self-reported as White, 2% as Asian, and 12% as other (e.g., Black or Hispanic).

Experiment 1: Long-Term Recognition Memory Deficits

In Experiment 1, we sought to replicate the well-known ORE, wherein people excel at recognising faces from their own race while being relatively poor at recognising faces from another race (usually showing inflated false alarms). To this end, we conducted a discrete recognition memory experiment wherein participants studied a series of faces before making delayed recognition ments. A secondary goal of conducting Experiment 1 was validate a new set of stimuli: To provide tight experimental for the match-to-sample tests, we developed a set of realistic stimuli, which we could manipulate and present in lieu of graphic quality faces. As such, one of our aims in Experiment 1 was to ensure that our new stimuli were processed in the general manner as actual faces (i.e., we wanted to verify computerized faces evoke the ORE, like more naturalistic sions). Our computerized faces were all derived from actual tographs (sources are noted below). To validate our stimuli acceptable proxy for real faces, we tested all participants with the original photographs and their computerized versions. sets of stimuli were presented in separate, but otherwise experiments, divided by a distractor task. We expected to find recognition deficit for Asian faces, relative to recognition of White faces, in both the real and computerized versions of the task.

Method

Participants. Thirty-six participants were recruited from Arizona State University introductory psychology subject pool. They participated in exchange for partial course credit.

Stimulus materials. Original photographs of faces were gathered from the PAL database (Minear & Park, 2004), the FERET database2 (Phillips, Moon, Rizvi, & Rauss, 2000; Phillips, Wechsler, Huang, & Rauss, 1998), and the Ekman database (Ekman & Matsumoto, 1993). We selected sets of 80 Asian and 80 White faces, half male and half female, and generated computerized versions of each face using FaceGen Modeller software (Singular Inversions, Inc., 2004). This software equates the faces for size and allows modifications along 61 shape dimensions (e.g., brow-nose-chin ratio) and 36 texture dimensions (e.g., pale/flush) to more closely approximate the original face (see Figure 1). All original photographs were neutral in expression and lacked extraneous detail, such as glasses or facial hair. After generating the computerized faces, all the original photographs were edited with Microsoft Picture It software. Each face was cropped to remove most of the hair and resized to match the computerized versions. All faces, both photographic and computerized, were saved against a uniform background of grey (RGB = 140).

Design and procedure. Participants were asked to study 32 faces, 16 Asian and 16 White, for an upcoming recognition task. Stimulus type (real vs. computerized faces) was manipulated within-subjects, but between tasks (i.e., participants were not exposed to both types of face during a single task). Both memory tasks proceeded identically, as follows. Participants were instructed to press the spacebar on a "ready" screen to view each face, one at a time. After 3,200 ms had elapsed, the face disappeared and was replaced by the ready screen for the next trial. After all 32 faces had been studied, participants were given a 4 min break, during which time they were allowed to read, fill out puzzles, or simply sit quietly (most participants text messaged on their cell phones). Following the break, participants were shown 64 faces (half "old") for a recognition task. "Old" and "new" responses were indicated via keypress and faces were presented individually in a response-terminated display. Feedback was not provided, and faces were presented in random order.

Following the first memory task, all participants completed a "filler" task, which served as pilot data for another researcher in our lab. This was a lexical decision experiment using handwritten stimuli and lasted approximately 15 min. After the filler task was complete, participants completed the second face memory task, with the stimulus set not used previously. The presentation order of the stimulus sets was counterbalanced across participants.

Results and Discussion

Throughout all analyses reported in this article, a significance criterion of α = .05 was adopted. Multiple comparisons were corrected for Type I error with Bonferonni adjustments. In Experiment 1, our analyses focused on signal-detection measures of sensitivity and bias. For each participant, we calculated d' and Pn two measures of sensitivity, the former being the classic signaldetection measure and the latter from the "two-high threshold" model of recognition, representing the difference between hits (i.e., responding "old" when the face is old) and false alarms (i.e., responding "old" when the face is new; Feenan & Snodgrass, 1990; see Kleider & Goldinger, 2004). Also from the two-high threshold model, we calculated B^ which is a measure of bias, defined as the probability of responding "old" despite uncertainty (B^sub r^ = FAI(I - P^sub r^)). B^sub r^ is centered around .5, with lower values reflecting a conservative bias.

The memory data were analysed in 10 repeated measures, oneway analyses of variances (ANOVAs), comparing race (Asian/ White) and contrasting stimulus (photo/computerized), for each of the five measurement types (hits, false alarms, d', P^sub r^, and B^sub r^). In the interest of brevity, means for each measurement by stimulus type and race are shown in Table 1, and significant mean differences are noted.

As can be seen in the table, participants generally performed better in response to White faces, relative to Asian faces.3 Although hits did not differ between the races, false alarms were approximately 10% higher to Asian faces (both photographic and computerized). This was reflected in other measures of sensitivity and bias, indicating that participants were more liberal (B^sub r^), less sensitive (d'), and had closer hit and false alarm rates (P^sub r^) when judging Asian faces, relative to judging White faces. For the photographic faces, we observed numerical trends toward higher sensitivity for own-race faces, and a reliable difference in bias. For the computerized faces, we observed robust differences in sensitivity, without a difference in bias. These different statistical profiles reflect small differences in hit rates. Critically, the ORE was observed in both stimulus sets, suggesting that the ORE is reliably observed in response to photographic-quality faces and to computerized versions of those faces. Although the patterns of results across stimulus types were not identical, the data followed the same general trend, and the classic ORE is traditionally reflected in inflated false alarms.4 Thus, we used only the computerized faces in our subsequent experiments, allowing us to balance our stimuli while enacting various manipulations.

Experiments 2 Through 4: Match-to-Sample Tasks

Experiments 2 through 4 were designed to determine whether the recognition deficit associated with the ORE is the result of immediate, encoding-based differences, or other, postperceptual factors. As was previously established, the ORE occurs when people study, and later try to recognise, other-race faces. Research also has shown immediate perceptual effects related to the ORE, such as differences in visual search (Levin, 1996, 2000), lightness perception (Levin & Banaji, 2006), and colour perception (Papesh & Goldinger, 2009). As noted earlier, Lindsay et al. (1991) found an other-race deficit in an immediate memory task, utilising a match-to-sample procedure. Participants in their experiment were exposed to masked prime faces for 120 ms each and then tried to make two-alternative forced-choice (2-AFC) decisions, choosing the targets over matched foils. The foils were paired to the targets based on the intuitions of a White researcher and a Black student (their study contrasted White and Black face recognition), leaving target-foil similarity subjectively controlled. Overall, in Experiments 2 through 4, we sought to replicate this effect with Asian and White faces, using objectively controlled stimuli. Simply stated, in each trial of Experiments 2 through 4, participants were presented with a prime face for study, and after a variable interstimulus interval (ISI), were shown the same face, now designated as the target, and a foil. Target-foil similarity was manipulated within and across experiments and, because of the manner in which we created the stimuli (see below), lightness and skin tone could not be used as reliable cues for recognition.

General Method

Participants. A total of 117 participants, recruited from the Arizona State University introductory psychology subject pool, participated in Experiments 2 through 4, in exchange for partial course credit. The precise numbers of participants per experiment are listed in Table 2. No participants were included in more than one experiment.

Stimulus materials. The computerized faces generated for Experiment 1 were used in all remaining experiments. In each trial of Experiments 2 through 4, participants saw a prime face, followed by that same face (as a target) and a matched foil: The matched foils were generated by systematically distorting each of the original 160 faces from Experiment 1 (in the interest of space, we will henceforth refer to computerized faces only as "faces"). Specifically, from each original face, we generated five distorted versions, yielding a total of 800 stimuli. The levels of distortion were increased, in a stepwise fashion, allowing us to make the discrimination task more or less challenging across trials. In creating distortions, we did not wish to consistently alter any single feature, which might become too salient over trials. Instead, we changed combinations of features, in three different pairs. Each distortion type involved simultaneously altering two facial features, eyesnose, eyes-mouth, or mouth-nose (see Figure 2).

The modifications were made by concurrently altering the parameter values for the selected features on each individual face. Parameter values in the FaceGen software range between -10.00 and +10.00, and control the size of the features (e.g., larger/ smaller or wider/thinner) while maintaining realistic dimensions within the face contour. Because all faces were scanned from photographs of real people with intrinsic differences, each of the facial features began with different starting values across the faces. Nevertheless, from these starting points, all changes were quantitatively equal, with features modified in the same directions and degrees, relative to each original face. From the starting values, we modified the two features per distortion type by five different levels: two, three, four, five, and six points of difference. Half of the faces were positively distorted two of the five times, whereas the other half were positively distorted three of the five times. Each distortion type was used at least once per face, with the remaining two distortions assigned to random faces in a counterbalanced fashion, thereby allowing us to represent each distortion type equally throughout the experiments. Also, unlike Experiment 1, all faces were given a generic hairstyle to make them appear more realistic. All women received a chin-length hairstyle and all men received a short, standard hairstyle. Asian faces were given black hair and White faces were given blonde hair (as in Figure 2), but otherwise the styles were identical.

Design and procedure. Participants were tested in groups of 2 to 8 on individual computers in the same room, separated by dividers. All experiments were programmed and controlled using E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) and were presented on Gateway 15'' CRT monitors with resolution of 1024 � 768. Figure 3 provides a schematic outline of Experiments 2 through 6, although Experiments 5, 6 A, and 6B will be discussed separately.

Experiments 2 through 4 were procedurally similar to one another, and are therefore described together: All participants were initially shown a black fixation cross against a white background for 1,500 ms, followed by a prime face for 1,500 ms. Once the prime face disappeared, a variable ISI was initiated, with duration manipulated within subjects (see Table 2). During the ISI, the screen was blank. At the termination of the ISI, participants were presented with a 2-AFC screen, and they indicated which face had been presented as the prime.5 Morphs and original faces were used equally often as targets and foils, and each participant completed 160 trials. Faces in the 2-AFC task were presented in a responseterminated display. Further details on each experiment, as well as the logical progression from one experiment to the next, are provided in tandem with the results.

Results and Discussion

Data from Experiments 2 through 4 were analysed in separate 2 (race) � 3 (ISI) � 5 (morph: two/three/four/five/six levels of distortion) ANOVAs. Accuracy means for each race in the separate experiments are presented in Table 3, organised by ISI (short, medium, long). Unless otherwise noted, there were no significant interactions in the data. Overall, Experiments 2 through 4 did not elicit any reliable other-race recognition deficits. Each experiment will be considered briefly, in turn.

In Experiment 2, we varied the retention intervals before the decision screen with ISIs of 250, 500, and 750 ms. Neither the main effect of race nor ISI was reliable, with no hints toward effects on recognition, both Fs < 1.5. In fact, the only variable to affect recognition was the distortion level of the faces (morph), F(4, 11) = 23.84, p < .05, η^sup 2^^sub p^ = .90 (statistical power = 1.0). As is intuitively obvious, when foils were distorted to a greater degree, discrimination accuracy improved (see Figure 4).

In Experiment 3, we maintained the within-subjects manipulations of race and morph, and only altered the retention intervals between prime presentation and the 2-AFC screen, with ISIs of 250, 1,250, and 2,250 ms. Our reasoning (which also applied to the changes made in Experiment 4) was that extending the duration of the blank screen, which came after encoding but before test, might allow any latent ORE to gain strength. We hypothesised that, relative to the retention of own-race faces, participants would have greater difficulty maintaining accurate other-race face representations during longer retention intervals. As in Experiment 2, neither race nor ISI had a significant impact on accuracy, both Fs < 3.0. Again, the main effect of morph was significant, F(4, 38) = 68.21, p < .05, η^sup 2^^sub p^ = .88 (statistical power = 1.0), as recognition accuracy increased when targets and foils differed more clearly.

Undaunted, we again manipulated ISI duration in Experiment 4, leaving every other variable unchanged from Experiments 2 and 3. We now used longer ISIs, of 1,250, 2,250, and 3,250 ms. Once again, only the main effect of morph was significant, F(4, 56) = 71.54, p < .05, η^sup 2^^sub p^ = .84 (statistical power = 1.0). F values for both race and ISI were less than 1.5.

In contrast to Experiment 1, in which we observed an ORE after a retention interval of several minutes, we were repeatedly unable to observe race-based recognition deficits in Experiments 2 through 4, in which we varied ISI duration and target-foil similarity. Although it has been well-established that own- and other-race faces elicit differential processing (e.g., Lindsay et al., 1991; Walker & Hewstone, 2006; Walker & Tanaka, 2003), these processing differences do not seem to manifest themselves in an immediate recognition deficit. Despite prior findings, wherein immediate match-to-sample tasks elicited an ORE, our wellcontrolled stimuli were unable to elicit such an effect. Because of this, we are left to question whether the ORE in recognition memory is the result of encoding differences, as has been argued by Walker, Tanaka, Lindsay et al., and others, or whether they are an artefact of the inherent differences in the stimuli chosen for those experiments. That is, in Experiments 2 through 4, target-foil similarity had robust effects on performance. It is plausible that, in studies using natural faces, such similarity levels may be confounded with race. Indeed, discriminability differences are a key part of the ORE. Our experiments did not suffer this potential confound, as our stimuli were equated for differences between targets and foils across races and, as shown by the similar accuracy patterns across the distortion levels for each race, the distortions applied to the faces were psychologically equivalent across races. Recognition benefits increased, to equivalent degrees across both races, as morph levels increased.

Our results thus far suggest that, despite early perceptual differences that may arise in own- and other-race face processing, there is no apparent ORE in immediate face discrimination. Or, at any rate, we could not observe an effect, despite repeated attempts. To assess whether the effect can occur quickly, but just beyond face encoding, we conducted a final set of experiments, in which we found that target-foil dissimilarity and ISI interference are crucial for a recognition deficit to emerge.

Experiment 5: An Early, But Not Encoding-Based, Recognition Deficit

After repeatedly failing to demonstrate an ORE in immediate recognition tasks, we altered our experimental paradigm once more, to disrupt any potential processing during the retention interval. In Experiment 5, we only used one type of morph per experiment, a five-level distortion, reflecting the fact that accuracy was good (~ 86%), but not at ceiling, for five-level distortions in the previous three experiments.6 In addition, because our ISI manipulation resulted in impressively null effects on recognition accuracy, we deleted this variable. In place of a blank ISI screen, we inserted multiple-choice questions, again aiming to disrupt participants' processing. We hypothesised that distracting participants would induce greater forgetting of facial detail, which would allow the ORE to emerge in recognition.

Method

Participants. We recruited 44 students from the Arizona State University introductory psychology subject pool, none of whom had completed any of the previous experiments.

Stimulus materials. We used a subset of same materials from the preceding experiments, specifically the original faces and their five-level morphed counterparts.

Procedure. The procedure was similar to that used in Experiments 2 through 4, but the ISI was now filled with multiple-choice academic and trivia questions (e.g., How many U.S. presidents have been assassinated while in office?), and therefore varied on a per-participant/per-trial basis. Briefly, participants viewed a prime face for 1,500 ms, which then disappeared and was replaced by the trivia question. After answering the trivia question, participants were presented with a 2-AFC screen containing the prime and a matched foil (see Figure 3). Responses were indicated via keypress.

Results and Discussion

The results were analysed in two paired-samples t tests, examining reaction time (RT) and accuracy. RTs to Asian faces (2,247 ms) and White faces (2,233 ms) were statistically equivalent, i(l, 44) = -2.77, p > .05. Although there were no race-based RT differences, we observed an ORE in accuracy, with better recognition for White faces (.71), relative to Asian faces (.66), f(l, 44) = -2.77, p < .05, Cohen's d = -.49 (Cohen, 1977). The results from Experiment 5 suggest that, for an ORE to emerge in recognition memory, the retention interval needs to be filled with a distraction, perhaps forcing degradation in the mental representation of other-race faces. Own-race faces, on the other hand, are apparently less affected by the disruption during the retention interval, demonstrating greater resistance to interference. In addition, target-foil similarity appears to be a major determinant in the emergence of the ORE. If the faces are too similar (e.g., level-two distortions), performance will be poor and nearly equivalent to faces from both races. If, however, the differences between target and foil faces are subtle, yet still discriminable (e.g., level-five distortions), participants will be able to make this discrimination more easily with own-race, versus other-race, faces.

Experiments 6A and 6B: Replications and Extensions

Although Experiment 5 confirmed the hypothesis that differences in memory for other-race faces are driven by processes beyond initial encoding, the effect was modest. As such, we conducted another two experiments, aimed to determine whether these effects would replicate, and whether the differences observed were truly the result of the disruptive trivia questions or if they would be observed without disruption. We hypothesised that participants would demonstrate poorer memory for other-race faces when their retention was disrupted by trivia, relative to an equivalent empty waiting period, potentially reflecting reliance on longterm memory for face matching.

Method

Participants. Thirty-four White participants were recruited from the Arizona State University introductory psychology subject pool and were randomly assigned to Experiment 6A (trivia) or Experiment 6B (no trivia). Five participants were dropped from analysis for consistent below-chance performance, leaving 17 participants in Experiment 6 A and 12 in Experiment 6B.

Stimulus materials. Both experiments used the stimuli from Experiment 5 (original faces and their five-level morphed counterparts).

Procedure. The experiments were run between-subjects, following the trial-by-trial procedure of Experiment 5. Experiments 6A and 6B differed in one key regard: Whereas 6A was a direct replication of Experiment 5, Experiment 6B was a modified replication, replacing the retention interval trivia questions with a 5,280-ms blank screen, which was the average RT to the same set of trivia questions by a group of six pilot test volunteers).

Results and Discussion

Accuracy data from both experiments were analysed in a 2 (Experiment: 6A/6B) � 2 (race) mixed model, repeated measures ANOVA.7 Overall, there was no race effect, F < 1.0, and there was a marginal effect of experiment, F(I, 27) = 3.81, p = .06, η^sup 2^^sub p^ = .16 (statistical power = .59). Participants who answered trivia questions before issuing 2-AFC decisions had an average accuracy of .73, whereas participants who did not answer trivia questions had an average accuracy of .79.

Planned comparisons examining the relationship between experiment and race indicate that differences in Experiment were only observed for the Asian faces, F(I, 27) = 11.13, p < .05, η^sup 2^^sub p^ = .29 (statistical power = .90). When participants were required to answer trivia questions during the retention interval, they were an average of 10% less accurate to Asian faces, relative to when they did not have to answer questions (.71 vs. .81). Accuracy for White faces was statistically equal across experiments, F < 1.0. Experiments 6A and 6B provide evidence that the difference observed in Experiment 5 was reliable and the result of processing disruption during retention. If the differences resulted from encoding, they would have been observed in Experiment 6B, and the questions would not have had such a detrimental effect on performance in Experiment 6A.

General Discussion

In six experiments, we found evidence suggesting that the ORE in recognition memory is functionally distinct from OREs occurring early in perceptual processes (e.g., distortions in perceived lightness or colour, facilitated visual search). In Experiment 1, Asian faces yielded the typical recognition deficit, in comparison to White faces, following a retention interval of several minutes. A similar ORE in Experiment 1 was observed with computergenerated faces, validating their use in the 2-AFC tasks. In Experiments 2 through 4, we manipulated retention intervals and targetfoil similarity (see Table 2 for a summary of manipulations by experiment): None of our manipulations had any impact on immediate face recognition in a match-to-sample task, which is known to result in an ORE (Lindsay et al., 1991). In Experiments 5 and 6A, we used target and foil faces that were more discriminable than the majority effaces used in the preceding experiments and we distracted participants with trivia questions during the retention interval. These manipulations caused the ORE to reemerge in recognition accuracy. Experiment 6B highlighted the importance of distraction during the retention intervals in Experiments 5 and 6A: Comparing this experiment to the previous two indicated that only Asian faces were affected by the trivia questions; White face recognition accuracy remained stable. Of interest, using the same high-level distorted faces in Experiments 2 through 4 did not elicit an ORE. Although these experiments were procedurally similar to Experiments 5 and 6A, participants were not distracted during the retention intervals of those experiments, which may have prevented the emergence of the ORE. Overall, the results suggest that the ORE is the product of retention or retrieval, not a deficit in encoding.

Our results contrast with those of Lindsay et al. (1991), who found an ORE (amongst White participants) in a match-to-sample task contrasting Black and White face recognition. By their account, race-based asymmetries in face recognition could reflect intrinsic differences in the difficulty of the selected faces or real differences in the perceptual skills of participants. One potential limitation of the present research is that we were unable to represent White and Asian participants equally in our sample, a practise often used to study cross-race effects. This approach is typically used to ensure that, should an ORE be observed, it is not an artefact of inherent differences between the stimulus sets chosen for each race. Although we primarily sampled White participants, we were able to objectively equate the difficulty of the own-race and other-race trials, by using the same morph levels for the foil faces in each race. That is, each target face was presented with its own morphed counterpart, such that the difficulty of discrimination was quantitatively equal in Asian and White trials. Therefore, even if our sets of faces were not equally discriminable at the group level, it should not have affected our results in the matchto-sample tasks.

If our results reflected intrinsic differences in the difficulty of the chosen stimulus sets, we would have observed poor recognition abilities uniformly across our experiments. Instead, we observed relatively good performance to White faces and simultaneously poor performance to Asian faces, but only in tasks that tapped (relatively) long-term memory. Unlike Lindsay et al. (1991), we did not observe any clear race effects in our match-to-sample tasks, until Experiments 5 and 6A. Although our original motivations are not particularly relevant, we should note that our goal in this investigation was, in fact, to replicate the ORE in a test of immediate memory. In pursuit of this goal, we repeatedly tried to titrate performance, predicting the effect would emerge, but we could not make it happen. We observed this null effect in over 200 participants, leading us to conclude that the ORE in immediate recognition is elusive, at best.

Despite our inability to elicit immediate recognition deficits in other-race faces, we did observe a relatively early recognition deficit in Experiment 5, and its replication in Experiment 6A. Although these two experiments used a similar time-course as the experiments by Lindsay et al. (1991), we do not attribute the ORE to an immediate perceptual effect. Because we disrupted participants' rehearsal and maintenance during the retention interval, we may have forced them to rely on long-term representations when they made 2-AFC decisions. Because the similarity between the target and foil faces was equated between the Asian and White faces, we believe that our results reflect the inability to appreciate subtle changes in other-race faces, although such detail is preserved in the representation of own-race faces. This explanation fits nicely within the framework of multidimensional space (MDS) accounts of the ORE (Byatt & Rhodes, 2004; Valentine, 1991; Valentine & Endo, 1992). According to MDS accounts, people are ill-equipped to distinguish subtle differences amongst other-race faces, while being simultaneously expert at appreciating such differences in own-race faces. When faces are stored within a hypothetical "face space," they are arranged along dimensions useful for discriminating amongst stimuli. The points representing own-race faces are sparsely distributed throughout this space, reflecting the perceiver's enhanced ability to recognise subtle featural or configurai changes. Points representing other-race faces, however, are more densely clustered, often toward the edges of the space, reflecting the perceiver's inability to accurately differentiate amongst them. Our results accord nicely with this explanation, as equivalent degrees of target-foil distortion across the races resulted in different levels of accuracy.

The ORE in recognition memory seems to emerge from longterm memory processing, not from differences in immediate encoding (although such differences clearly exist; Levin, 2000). The current experiments have shown that the ORE is near impossible to elicit in match-to-sample tasks when the similarity between target and foil faces is controlled across races. The effect only emerged when postperceptual processes, such as retention and rehearsal, were disrupted by a distraction task (in our experiments, we used lexical decision or trivia questions). Although our experiments do not represent an exhaustive set of immediate-memory tests of the ORE, we expended considerable effort in an attempt to elicit the effect, to no avail. The effect seems to emerge in the process of remembering or retrieving faces, and future research should be guided toward determining the precise time course of this wellknown effect.

[Sidebar]

R�sum�

L'effet de l'autre race (EAR) dans la reconnaissance des visages est typiquement observ� dans les t�ches requ�rant la m�moire � long terme. Cependant, plusieurs �tudes ont permis d'observer l'effet t�t dans l'encodage du visage (Lindsay, Jack, & Christian, 1991; Walker & Hewstone, 2006). Dans 6 exp�riences, avec plus de 300 participants, nous n'avons trouv� aucune preuve que le d�ficit de la reconnaissance associ� � l'EAR refl�te des d�ficits dans l'encodage imm�diat. Dans l'Exp�rience 1, avec un intervalle de r�tention de 4 minutes entre l'encodage et le test, les participants ont �t� meilleurs pour reconna�tre les visages blancs que les visages asiatiques. L'Exp�rience 1 a aussi valid� l'utilisation de visages g�n�r�s par ordinateurs dans les exp�riences suivantes. Dans les Exp�riences 2 � 4, la performance a �t� pratiquement identique avec les visages asiatiques et blancs dans une t�che de reconnaissance imm�diate d'appariement. Dans l'Exp�rience 5, le fait de diminuer la similarit� entre la cible et l'image encod�e et d'interrompre l'intervalle de r�tention en posant des questions quiz a entra�n� une r�mergence de l'EAR. Les Exp�riences 6A et 6B ont permis de r�pliquer cet effet et de montrer que la m�moire des visages asiatiques �tait particuli�rement susceptible � la distraction; les visages blancs ont �t� reconnus aussi bien, peu importe la pr�sence de questions quiz durant l'intervalle de r�tention. Le d�ficit de reconnaissance dans l'EAR �merge apparemment de d�ficits dans la r�tention ou le rappel et non de diff�rences dans le traitement perceptif imm�diat.

Mots-cl�s : reconnaissance du visage, effet de l'autre race, m�moire

[Reference]

References

Anthony, T., Copper, C., & Mullen, B. (1992). Cross-racial facial identification: A social cognitive integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 296-301.

Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittlinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experiment Psychology: General, 104, 54-75.

Bothwell, R. K., Brigham, J. C., & Malpass, R. S. (1989). Cross-racial identification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 19-25.

Brigham, J. C., & Barkowitz, P. (1978). Do "they all look alike?" The effect of race, sex, experience, and attitudes on the ability to recognize faces. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 306-318.

Byatt, G., & Rhodes, G. (2004). Identification of own-race and other-race faces: Implications for the representation of race in face-space. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 735-741.

Chance, J., Goldstein, A. G., & McBride, L. (1975). Differential experience and recognition memory for faces. The Journal of Social Psychology, 97, 243-253.

Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for behavioral sciences (Rev. ed.). New York: Academic.

Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1986). Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 107-117.

Eberhardt, J. L., Dasgupta, N., & Banaszynski, T. L. (2003). Believing is seeing: The effects of racial labels and implicit beliefs on face perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 360-370.

Ekman, P., & Matsumoto, D. (1993). Japanese and Caucasian neutral faces (JACNeuF) [Photographs on CD-Rom]. San Francisco, CA.

Farah, M. J., Wilson, K. D., Tanaka, J. N., & Drain, M. (1998). What is "special" about face perception? Psychological Review, 105, 482-498.

Feenan, K., & Snodgrass, J. G. (1990). The effect of context on discrimination and bias in recognition memory for pictures and words. Memory & Cognition, 18, 515-527.

Ferguson, G. P., Rhodes, D., Lee, K., & Sriram, N. (2001). "They all look alike to me": Prejudice and cross-race face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 567-577.

Gauthier, I., & Bukach, C. (2007). Should we reject the expertise hypothesis? Cognition, 103, 322-330.

Gibson, E. J. (1969). Principles of perceptual learning and development. New York: Appleton.

Goldstein, A. G. (1979). Race-related variation of facial features: Anthropometric data I. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 13, 187-190.

Goldstein, A. G., & Chance, J. E. (1980). Memory for faces and schema theory. The Journal of Psychology, 105, 47-59.

Hancock, K. J., & Rhodes, G. (2008). Contact, configurai coding and the other-race effect in face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 45-56.

Hayward, W. G., Rhodes, G., & Schwaninger, A. (2008). An own-race advantage for components as well as configurations in face recognition. Cognition, 106, 1017-1027.

Kleider, H. M., & Goldinger, S. D. (2004). Illusions of face memory: Clarity breeds familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 196-211.

Levin, D. T. (1996). Classifying faces by race: The structure of face categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1364-1382.

Levin, D. T. (2000). Race as a visual feature: Using visual search and perceptual discrimination tasks to understand face categories and the cross-race recognition deficit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 559-574.

Levin, D. T., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Distortions in the perceived lightness of faces: The role of race categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 501-512.

Lindsay, D. S., Jack, P. C., Jr., & Christian, M. A. (1991). Other-race face perception. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 587-589.

MacLin, O. H., & Malpass, R. S. (2001). Racial categorization of faces: The ambiguous race face effect. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 98-118.

Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: A meta-analytic review. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 3-35.

Michel, C, Caldara, R., & Rossion, B. (2006). Same-race faces are perceived more holistically than other-race faces. Visual Cognition, 14, 55-73.

Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, S.-C., & Caldara, R. (2006). Holistic processing is finely tuned for faces of one's own race. Psychological Science, 17, 608-615.

Minear, M., & Park, D. C. (2004). A lifespan database of adult facial stimuli. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 630-633.

Ng, W., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1994). Cross-race facial recognition: Failure of the contact hypothesis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 25, 217-232.

O'Toole, A. J., Deffenbacher, K. A., Valentin, D., & Abdi, H. (1994). Structural aspects of face recognition and the other-race effect. Memory & Cognition, 22, 208-224.

Ostrom, T. M., Carpenter, S. L., Sedikides, C., & U, F. (1993). Differential processing of in-group and out-group information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 21-34.

Papesh, M. H., & Goldinger, S. D. (2008). Visual search for racially ambiguous faces: A test of the race-feature hypothesis. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Phillips, P. J., Moon, H., Rizvi, S. A., & Rauss, P. J. (2000). The FERET evaluation methodology for face recognition algorithms. IEEE Trans Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 22, 1090-1 104.

Phillips, P. J., Wechsler, H., Huang, J., & Rauss, P. (1998). The FERET database and evaluation procedure for face recognition algorithms. Image and Vision Computing Journal, 16, 295-306.

Rakover, S. S. (2002). Featural vs. configurational information in faces: A conceptual and empirical analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 1-30.

Rhodes, G., Tan, S., Brake, S., & Taylor, K. (1989). Expertise and configurai coding in face recognition: Erratum. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 526.

Robbins, R., & McKone, E. (2007). No face-like processing for objectsof-expertise in three behavioural tasks. Cognition, 103, 34-79.

Singular Inversions, Inc. (2004). FaceGenModeller (Version 3.1.4) [Computer software]. Toronto, ON, Canada. Available: http://www.FaceGen .com

Slone, A., Brigham, J., & Meissner, C. (2000). Social and cognitive factors affecting the own-race bias in Whites. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 71-84.

Sporer, S. L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups: An integration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7, 36-97.

Swope, T. M. (1994). Social experience, illusory correlation and facial recognition ability. Unpublished master's thesis, Florida State University.

Tanaka, J. W., Kiefer, M., & Bukach, C. M. (2004). A holistic account of the own-race effect in face recognition: Evidence from a cross-cultural study. Cognition, 93, B1-B9.

Triesman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97-136.

Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43A., 161-204.

Valentine, T., & Endo, M. (1992). Towards an exemplar model of face processing: The effects of race and distinctiveness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 671-703.

Walker, P. M., & Hewstone, M. (2006). A perceptual discrimination investigation of the own-race effect and intergroup experience. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 461-475.

Walker, P. M., & Tanaka, J. W. (2003). An encoding advantage for own-race versus other-race faces. Perception, 32, 1117-1125.

Received: June 9, 2008

Accepted: February 18, 2009

[Author Affiliation]

Megan H. Papesh and Stephen D. Goldinger

Arizona State University

[Author Affiliation]

Megan H. Papesh and Stephen D. Goldinger, Department of Psychology, Arizona State University.

Support provided by U.S. National Institutes of Health Grant ROlDC04535 to Stephen D. Goldinger.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephen D. Goldinger, Department of Psychology, Box 871104, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104. E-mail: goldinger@asu.edu